IEPadvocate4you

Carol Sadler
Special Education Consultant/Advocate
770-442-8357
1105 Rock Pointe Look
Woodstock, GA 30188
CarolSadler@bellsouth.net
www.IEPadvocate4you.com

I am a lay Parent Advocate assisting parents of children with disabilities in school IDEA, 504 and SST meetings. I am a former CHADD and LDA Coordinator, graduate of the 1st GA Advocacy Office PLSP legal training course and most importantly parent of two children with various disabilities.

Wednesday, April 06, 2005

Current Status of Treatments for Dyslexia: Critical Review

FYI - This is one of the best most comprehensive articles I have ever seen on Dyslexia!! Lindamood-Bell is referenced to frequently, and is my favorite reading program to remediate dyslexia (and dyscalia). www.lindamoodbell.org

This portion of the article describes my daughter's severe dyslexia to a tee.........
Rapid word retrieval is often a factor in inefficient reading and is thought to be the result of deficits in phonologic, morphologic, and semantic processing; attention; executive function; articulatory processing; and/or a weak visual-verbal connection.[22,72] There is presumed to be an interplay between all of these neural components, so that a breakdown in any one could result in problems. Wolf et al. pointed out that these same networks subserve reading and might account for the word retrieval deficits being found in the poorest readers.[22] Children with these retrieval deficits progress more poorly and have significant difficulty with sight word reading, especially with fluency.

***The most prevalent comorbid condition impacting the development of the individual with dyslexia is attention deficit.

Carol Sadler
Special Education Consultant/Advocate
GA Advocacy Office PLSP I Graduate
770-442-8357
1105 Rock Pointe Look
Woodstock, GA 30188
CarolSadler@bellsouth.net
www.IEPadvocate4You.com
http://iepadvocate4you.blogspot.com/



You must register with Medscape, it is quick, easy, and free. Below is the first 4 pages of the 14 page article. Register to see the rest of the article!!

CURRENT STATUS OF TREATMENTS FOR DYSLEXIA: CRITICAL REVIEW
http://mp.medscape.com/cgi-bin1/DM/y/enhg0HYuTy0D2I0GXml0EI

From Journal of Child Neurology
Current Status of Treatments for Dyslexia: Critical Review
Ann W. Alexander, MD;Anne-Marie Slinger-Constant, MD

Abstract and Introduction
Abstract

The acquisition of reading is a complex neurobiologic process. Identifying the most effective instruction and remedial intervention methods for children at risk of developing reading problems and for those who are already struggling is equally complex. This article aims to provide the clinician with a review of more current findings on the prevention and remediation of reading problems in children, along with an approach to considering the diagnosis and treatment of a child with dyslexia. The first part of the review describes interventions targeted at preventing reading difficulties in the at-risk younger child. The second part of the review discusses the efficacy of approaches to treat the older, reading-disabled child ("intervention studies"). Factors that impact the response to treatment are also discussed, as are neuroimaging studies that offer insight into how the brain responds to treatment interventions. With appropriate instruction, at-risk readers can become both accurate and fluent readers. In contrast, although intensive, evidence-based remedial interventions can markedly improve reading accuracy in older, reading-disabled children, they have been significantly less effective in closing the fluency gap. Owing to the dynamic course of language development and the changes in language demands over time, even after a child has demonstrated a substantial response to treatment interventions, his or her subsequent progress should be carefully tracked to ensure optimal progress toward the development of functional reading and written language skills.

Introduction

Over the last 15 to 20 years, there has been a great deal of research focused on finding the most effective methods for treating reading disability (dyslexia). We have had the opportunity to evaluate not only the effect of specific interventions on reading skill but also to devise technologies that make it possible to study the way in which the brain responds to these interventions. This body of knowledge is complex, in part because although all individuals with dyslexia have a similar problem, namely, difficulty reading, they have heterogeneous characteristics, and depending on the child's developmental level, the demands of reading and the required skills are quite different. As Torgesen pointed out, even the definition of a good reader varies.Is it being able to orally decode accurately and fluently? Is it simply being able to get the gist of what is read? Or is it being able to analyze, synthesize, and convert information from multiple sources into a meaningful whole?[1] Torgesen noted that all of these factors have contributed to the challenge of designing intervention studies, interpreting the outcomes of treatment, and answering the following crucial questions:
What types of prevention or intervention treatment will be most effective? The answer depends on what children need to know to read at a given grade level and the reading level expected for their developmental stage (eg, mechanics of word reading, fluency, or text reading and comprehension of reading).
What level of intensity is most effective? There are two types of intensity: one referring to the frequency of the intervention (daily, two to three times per week, once per week) and the other referring to the instructor to student ratio (1:1, small group, classroom).
How many hours are needed to complete the intervention? What is the optimal duration of treatment?
How well are gains and skills maintained after the intervention has ended?
What therapist or teacher skills are needed?
What child characteristics contribute to the success or failure of the intervention (type of language involvement, severity, age, and comorbidity)?
In what educational context can these interventions be implemented?
To address some of these questions, the National Research Council, the research arm of the National Academy of Science, published a report in 1998 summarizing available research findings that could be used to prevent reading difficulties in young children.[2] The report emphasized the importance of early childhood language and literacy experience as the foundation on which systematic phonologic decoding should be taught. To explore how reading research could be more specifically applied to classroom implementation, the National Institute for Child Health and Human Development and the Department of Education convened the National Reading Panel to conduct a meta-analysis of reading research since 1990 in the areas of alphabetics, fluency, comprehension, teacher education, and the effects of computer technology. Only studies meeting rigorous research methodologic criteria were analyzed. Although much of the research in the field did not meet these criteria, and in some instances there were too few studies to conduct a meta-analysis, the National Reading Panel did find robust evidence in the areas of alphabetics (phonologic awareness, reading and spelling skills).[3]

Specifically, direct and systematic phonologic awareness and phonics instruction produced significant effects for at-risk readers (ie, young children in kindergarten or first grade who have had minimal exposure to reading and are deficient in phonologic awareness and letter knowledge—precursors for the acquisition of phonologic decoding), as well as disabled readers (those who have had exposure to adequate reading instruction and have not learned to read). The panel noted that there were differences in the type of instruction and the response of these two groups of children to instruction:
One type of instruction involved the prevention of reading disability in the young, at-risk child.
The younger the child (kindergarten through first grade), the better the outcome.
The at-risk child responds best to small-group instruction (2:1 or 3:1), with phonologic awareness training being combined with letter knowledge and explicit phonics instruction.
Trained teachers achieved good results.
More frequent instruction (4–5 days/week) was more effective.
Gains were maintained in most of the children at long-term follow-up.
The following characteristics of the child were associated with poor outcome in reading and spelling.poor rapid naming, poor verbal ability, and attention or behavior overall. Low socioeconomic status was also associated with poor spelling achievement. Computer programs were helpful aids.

The second type of instruction, remediation, was directed at the older, reading-disabled child (second to sixth grade):
Although the older, reading-disabled children responded with improved word reading to similar intensive, direct, and explicit instruction, they were less responsive, and gains were not as marked.
They did better with one-to-one or small-group instruction.
More intensive work for a longer duration was required.
Spelling and fluency did not respond well, but there was some improvement in reading comprehension.
Gains were maintained in most children at follow-up.

Characteristics that impeded reading and spelling gains included poor attention and behavior control, rapid naming deficits, and weak verbal ability. Low socioeconomic status affected reading gains; trained teachers achieved good results, but they were typically not as robust as the results when working with researchers. Computer instruction served as an effective aid but was not effective by itself.

The skills needed for normal reading development were found to encompass five essential domains:
Phonemic awareness.awareness of the sound structure of spoken language and the basic units of speech (phonemes)
Phonics.knowledge of relationships between letters and sounds and spelling-sound correspondences; allows fluent phonologic decoding
Sight word acquisition.automaticity of reading words by sight without having to decode sound by sound; leads to orthographic decoding fluency
Vocabulary.the storage of word meanings and the ability to fluently access that information
Comprehension of text.the ability to think about and extract the information provided in text while reading, a fluent integration of multiple processes.

Although the National Reading Panel's review of the research literature answered some of the questions about word reading instruction, there were not enough data to draw firm conclusions about the best way to teach fluency, vocabulary, and text comprehension or to train teachers, illustrating the need for more stringent research in the field. Nonetheless, preliminary findings suggested that:

Fluency is better achieved by repeated guided oral reading than by silent reading practice.
Vocabulary instruction should be taught by both direct and indirect methods, with computer programs as adjuncts.

Comprehension is developed by fluent word reading, vocabulary strength, and a combination of strategies for helping the child connect with and think about the text.
The most rigorous methodologic studies resulted in very robust effect sizes for reading gains; however, even the studies with the weakest methodology also yielded significant gains in phonologic awareness. With the impetus to improve the quality of the experimental studies to arrive at more definitive answers to our questions, research has continued to advance our understanding of how children develop skilled reading and to learn more about the factors that hinder that development.[4] More recent findings are summarized below.

Current Research
This article focuses on research studies that compare treatment efficacy for prevention (ie, working with young children who are "at risk") and the efficacy of various approaches to remediation (ie, treating older children who have already been identified as reading disabled). Pre- and post-treatment functional brain imaging studies have been conducted in some of these studies, and these are discussed. Behavioral studies in combination with neuroimaging studies allow us to develop an understanding of the neurobiologic correlates of behavioral responses to treatment. Finally, development of computational models, computer simulations of how reading is acquired, has not only provided additional insight into the process of reading acquisition but has also offered explanations for specific intervention outcomes.[4]

Prevention Studies
Torgesen, in an overview of research on the effectiveness of interventions with at-risk children, in the classroom and with small groups, noted that two programs providing relatively direct, systematic instruction in phonemic decoding skills, "Direct Instruction" and "Success for All," were effective for this at-risk group but not for already impaired readers.[1] Tunmer and Chapman conducted a longitudinal study using the Reading Recovery Method, which has been a popular early intervention program in the schools.[5,6] It offers one-to-one, daily, pull-out tutoring for 12 to 20 weeks for 6-year-old children performing at or below the 30th percentile in reading. Although it has been reported to be helpful for struggling readers, its effectiveness has not been documented by rigorous research. Tunmer and Chapman found that 30% of the children were referred out of the program because they were not responding.[5] These children were found to have significant phonologic deficits, which probably accounts in part for their lack of response. When the children who did show a good response to the intervention at the end of the treatment were tested 1 year later, their performance was no better than that of the controls. These findings indicate that intensive treatment alone is not enough; despite one-to-one instruction, the at-risk children did not respond as well as those receiving appropriate classroom intervention. This study illustrates that the content or method of intervention is itself a critical factor in treatment efficacy. The Reading Recovery Method is a top-down, more whole-language approach using semantic or syntactic clues for word reading and does not contain the explicit phonics or phonologic awareness instruction needed by these younger at-risk children.

Denton and Mathes reported the effectiveness of four primary classroom treatment projects that followed the National Reading Panel recommendations.[7] The study targeted at-risk children who fell between the 18th and 25th percentiles in phonologic and letter knowledge skills. After intervention, 18 to 31% of the children still had not reached the benchmark of the 30th percentile for word-level reading and would require a higher level of intervention. Denton and Mathes also evaluated the effectiveness of five interventions that involved a more intensive approach (one to one, small group with more hours of instruction) with students who were more impaired (12–18th percentile). They found that 4 to 30% remained below the 30th percentile benchmark after this treatment. On extrapolating their findings to the general population for both groups, they noted that only 0.7 to 4.5% of the more severely impaired group and 5 to 6% of the mild to moderately impaired group remained in the impaired range, a significant difference from the 15 to 20% rate currently reported. Both types of intervention yielded significant results, with the more intensive administration moving even the more severely at-risk children into the average range.
Torgesen and colleagues conducted a study of kindergartners who were severely at risk (10th percentile for prereading skills).[8] The study compared the effect of three types of intensive interventions that were delivered.one on one, 20 minutes/day, and 4 days/week through the end of second grade. There was also a no-treatment control group. The interventions were (1) a regular classroom reading curriculum, which was more whole language in nature; (2) embedded phonics training, which provided more implicit, nonsequential phonics instruction when the opportunity presented itself in text; text reading and writing instruction were the predominant component; and (3) an explicit, sequential direct teaching of phonemic awareness using a multisensory approach, which brought in the motor perception of speech sounds (the Lindamood Auditory Discrimination in Depth program)[9] to facilitate the development of fine-grained phonemic representations. Decoding and encoding of words with direct phonics instruction comprised most of the instruction, with little emphasis on text reading. This approach was a predominantly bottom-up, sensory approach for the development of more distinct phoneme representations, in contrast to the more top-down orthographic-semantic approach of the other interventions. It also offered more explicit phonics instruction.

The bottom-up, sensory, more explicit approach was significantly superior to all of the other groups at the end of the intervention period. The more top-down interventions resulted in outcomes similar to those of the no-treatment controls. Long-term follow-up revealed that the more explicitly trained group (those receiving the Lindamood Auditory Discrimination in Depth program) was performing solidly in the average range for accuracy and fluency at the end of the fourth grade. These findings suggest that these young, impaired children required much more salient intervention to improve their phoneme maps, combined with explicit instruction for letter and sound mapping. The more implicit phonics approach that taught phonics rules was ineffective. Perhaps this was due to the more abstract, top-down nature of teaching the phonics rules, without establishing fine-grained phonemic representations. With deficient phonologic processing, less explicit instruction would place greater demands on executive function. Because neither executive function nor abstract thinking ability is fully developed at this younger age, a more concrete intervention program would be expected to be most effective for the struggling reader.

Denton and Mathes also addressed the question of which criteria are most useful for predicting success with future reading acquisition following early intervention.[7] They found that fluency (or words read per minute) was more significantly related to future outcome than other reading measures, and the rate of response to intervention was also a predictor of future reading development—the slower the learning curve, the more difficult it was for the child to become an adequate reader.

Vellutino and colleagues also found the rate of response to treatment to be a significant predictor of outcome. Their 6-year longitudinal study of at-risk first-graders revealed that the children in the "limited response to intervention" group (treatment resisters) differed from the children who were readily remediable.[10] Although all of the subjects had significant difficulty with phonologic skills such as phoneme awareness and with letter and number naming, there were differences between the readily remediated readers and the most difficult-to-remediate readers in their cognitive and language-based abilities.rapid naming, confrontational naming, verbal working memory (syntactic word order, nonword repetition), short-term verbal memory (digit span), and articulation speed. The readily remediated group performed more like the normal reading group on these measures. These children were more likely to have had limitations in their early language experiences and/or instruction. Thus, although all of the children had phonologically based deficits, there were two different types of deficient phonologic sensitivity. One group lacked the environmental exposure necessary for the development of a strong phonologic system (an "experiential reading disability"); the other group of children had been exposed to appropriate environmental stimulation but had atypical development of the neural systems underlying phonologic sensitivity (characteristic of the typical individual with dyslexia). With the appropriate input, the former group could develop the brain maps of the normal reader. The cognitive and linguistic deficits of the individual with dyslexia suggest a more pervasive difficulty, which is constitutional in nature and presents a distinct therapeutic challenge.

Intervention Studies
Intervention studies have demonstrated that intensive (daily, one on one, and small group), phonologically based treatments can close the gap for reading accuracy, even in those children falling as low as the 2nd percentile in word-level reading skills. In his intervention study, Torgesen and colleagues contrasted the two treatment approaches described in the prevention study above.(1) a multisensory, bottom–up, explicit approach for developing phonemic awareness and phonemic decoding and encoding skills with minimal text instruction (the Lindamood program, described above) and (2) the embedded phonics instruction approach in which only 20% of the time was spent on single-word phonemic decoding activities and the rest on text reading and sight word training.[9,11,12] The children were 8 to 11 years old and fell in the 2nd percentile for word-level reading ability and the 10th percentile on a broad reading measure, combining word reading with comprehension (Broad Reading Score, Woodcock Johnson Tests of Achievement, Revised).[13] Intensive remediation was delivered on a one-to-one basis, 5 days a week for 100 minutes a day for 8 weeks, and the children then returned to their special education classrooms.
Figure 1 provides a snapshot of the findings. The growth of the children's reading skills in both conditions was measured on a broad measure combining word reading accuracy and passage comprehension into a single standard score (mean 100, SD 15; thus, a value from 85 to 115 is in the average range).

Figure 1. (click image to zoom) Treatment effects.broad reading skill measures (Woodcock Johnson Test of Achievement, Third Edition, standard score mean 100, SD ± 15) before 16 months of special education (P-Pretest), before 9 weeks of intensive remediation (Pre), immediately after 9 weeks of remediation (Post), and at 1- and 2-year follow-up. The dotted horizontal line represents the group mean IQ. EP = embedded phonics; LIPS = Lindamood Phoneme Sequencing.

For the 16 months before the intervention, the children were in special education classes, where the instruction maintained their level but did not close the gap. The intensive intervention produced a steep rate of reading growth, regardless of the intervention, and gains continued over the 2 years, with the children reaching mean standard scores above 90, approximately the 30th percentile, which was also their mean Wechsler Verbal IQ. Even the children with the lowest verbal short-term memory (standard score 70–85) as measured by the Digit Span subtest of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R)[14] made gains in phonemic decoding similar to those of the group as a whole (mean standard score 70.2 to 93.5). At follow-up, 40% of the children had been staffed out of the special education classrooms, a considerably higher percentage than the county's record of less than 5%.

Whereas almost all of the children made significant gains during the intervention period, only a little more than half sustained or increased their gains during follow-up. Furthermore, about a fourth of the children lost most of the gains made during the intervention during the 2-year follow-up period. The variables predicting growth during the follow-up were attention, receptive language ability, and socioeconomic status. Of note, the more implicit top-down approach was as effective as the multisensory bottom-up approach, suggesting that the children could harness the phonics rules despite having weak phonologic representations. This would suggest that they now had the executive function and abstract thinking abilities to compensate.

Unfortunately, the fluency gap could not be narrowed with this intensive intervention, and the children remained severely impaired in reading rate (2nd percentile). To explore this further, Torgesen and colleagues conducted small-group intervention studies with older (11–12 years) reading-disabled children.[15] All children were severely impaired in word reading fluency (2nd percentile), but one group was mildly impaired (30th percentile) and the other moderately impaired (10th percentile) for word reading skills. The intervention used the Spell, Read Phonological Auditory Training,[16] which provided systematic instruction in phonemic awareness and phonemic decoding combined with fluency-oriented practice from the start. The mildly impaired group evidenced significant gains in both accuracy and fluency, with fluency improving from the 2nd percentile to approximately the 50th percentile after only 60 hours of treatment. The moderately impaired readers were further subdivided into two groups, which received different durations of treatment. One group received 50 hours of treatment, and the other received 100 hours. Although the gains in text reading accuracy and fluency on the Gray Oral Reading Test, Third Edition,[17] were significant in both groups, the longer instruction period produced greater gains in accuracy (19 vs 8 standard score point improvement) and fluency. The group exposed to the longer duration of treatment improved in fluency from a mean standard score of 65 to 79 (14 standard score points—nearly 1 SD), whereas the group exposed to a shorter duration of treatment showed a 9 standard score point improvement (68 to 78). Clearly, fluency remained a significant problem for both groups.

Figure 2 illustrates the results from the prevention study of the young at-risk children as well as the remediation study of the older, reading-disabled children.[15] Treatment of the young at-risk children in the prevention study resulted in normal accuracy and fluency at the end of fourth grade, emphasizing the importance of early identification and treatment.[8] Although the mildly impaired readers (30th percentile) experienced very significant growth after treatment, they still evidenced a gap between accuracy and fluency. The more severely impaired readers (10th percentile) made significant gains in accuracy but remained impaired in fluency. The longer duration of treatment in the moderately impaired readers resulted in more gains but was still inadequate. The most severely impaired (2nd percentile) readers from Torgesen et al's intervention study also made significant gains in accuracy but no gains in fluency.[11]

Figure 2. (click image to zoom) The fluency gap.a comparison of word-level fluency gains immediately after intensive, phonologically based treatment in groups of reading disabled (RD) children, with varying severity of difficulty prior to treatment. The two groups of RD children in the 10th percentile illustrate that longer treatment duration (Rx hrs) yielded greater gains in accuracy but no response with fluency. A contrast group of at-risk children, who had received intensive intervention through second grade, illustrates maintenance of gains through fourth grade, with normal fluency. The horizontal line represents the 30th percentile benchmark.
Torgesen and colleagues conducted a subsequent intervention study with two groups of severely impaired (2nd percentile) 9- to 11-year-old children using two interventions.(1) the Lindamood Phoneme Sequencing program[12] for 67.5 hours, followed by 67.5 hours of fluency instruction (repeated reading and word drills) and comprehension instruction, and (2) an accuracy-only group, which received only the Lindamood Phoneme Sequencing instruction, with equal time in comprehension instruction.[15] Preliminary findings based on 45 of the 60 children who finished the study reveal similar accuracy and fluency outcomes for both groups, with significant gains in accuracy but none in fluency. Neither fluency instruction nor a longer duration could close the gap.
The studies by Torgesen et al. support the need for early intervention for the development of fluent word reading. Torgesen proposed that because the acquisition of sight words occurs with repeated exposure to words in print, individuals with dyslexia do not read, and the fluency gap widens.[1] Those older children who are remediated and acquire the phonologic decoding ability for accurate reading would have to read more than other children to close the sight word gap. Other factors contributing to poor automatic word reading and text reading fluency can be child characteristics such as rapid naming, attention deficit, executive function deficits, or receptive language ability. In their 2001 study, Torgesen et al. found that attention, receptive language ability, and socioeconomic status were predictive factors in this population.[11] Indeed, the children in the 2001 study were found to have significant language impairment as measured by the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, Third Revision,[18] with a mean total language standard score of 76.3 (+ 9.0) for the group receiving the Lindamood Phoneme Sequencing, and of 81 (+ 12) in the embedded phonics group. Interestingly, these language-impaired children showed a significant improvement in the total language standard score at 1-year follow-up.the Lindamood Phoneme Sequencing group standard score improved to 89.7 (+ 14), and the embedded phonics group standard score improved to 89.9 (+ 19.3). Both the explicit and the more implicit phonologic interventions were effective in significantly enhancing spoken language processing and written language.

Pokorni et al. reported minimal gains in language and reading-related skills in a group of 18 younger, language-impaired, poor readers, aged 7.5 to 9 years.[19] Their scores on the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, Third Edition,[18] fell more than 1.5 SD below the mean, and they were moderately reading impaired (18–25th percentile). These children received the Lindamood Phoneme Sequencing intervention for approximately the same number of hours of daily treatment, but in a small group (four children to one instructor). Two other groups also received equal time and intensity of treatment using either Earobics Step II[20] or FastForWord,[21] computer programs designed to remediate auditory temporal processing deficits. The Lindamood Phoneme Sequencing group alone made significant gains in phonologic awareness, as well as segmenting and blending after 60 hours of intervention. However, no gains in reading were observed. This would suggest that the more intensive, one-on-one intervention is more effective, particularly for children with both oral and written language impairments. Moreover, Lindamood Phoneme Sequencing is a complex program, and considerable training is required to work effectively with very impaired children. It is possible that the training and experience of the therapists, which were not specified in this study, might not have been as extensive as reported in Torgesen et al's 2001 study,[11] and might have been a contributing factor. The authors cautioned that the study was limited by a small sample size, a heterogeneous group, and an absence of IQ scores.

In addressing the comprehensive and multidimensional needs of children with dyslexia, especially in the areas of oral (rapid naming) and reading fluency, Wolf et al. reported the preliminary findings of their longitudinal intervention study with reading-disabled children.[22] Interventions contrasted three different treatment strategies. All subjects were trained using an evidence-based phonologic treatment (Phonological Awareness and Blending/Direct Instruction, developed by Lovett et al.[23]) followed by three different protocols.(1) RAVE-O (Retrieval, Automaticity, Vocabulary Elaboration, Orthography), developed by Wolf et al. "to simultaneously address the need for automaticity in phonological, orthographic, semantic, syntactic, and morphological systems and the importance of training explicit connections between these linguistic system"[24] ; the comprehensive interventions with emphasis on the semantic aspects of language (vocabulary and retrieval) set it apart from other linguistic intervention studies; (2) Word Identification Strategy Training (teaching other strategies for analyzing words)[23] ; and (3) Teaching Classroom Survival Strategies.[22] Subjects were second- and third-graders. They received 70 sessions of small-group, daily interventions. Preliminary findings reported by Wolf et al. on the children with low average vocabulary scores revealed that the Phonological Awareness and Blending/Direct Instruction plus RAVE-O group improved more than the Phonological Awareness and Blending/Direct Instruction plus Word Identification Strategy Training group.[22] On the Gray Oral Reading Test, Third Edition, Reading Quotient (a combined measure of rate, accuracy, and comprehension subtests), the Phonological Awareness and Blending/Direct Instruction + RAVE-O group showed a 10-point standard score gain (74 to 84) compared with the Phonological Awareness and Blending/Direct Instruction plus Word Identification Strategy Training group, which showed only a 4-point standard score improvement (74 to 78). However, at the 1-year follow-up, both groups were performing similarly, in the low 80s (Lovett M, personal communication, 2004). The Phonological Awareness and Blending/Direct Instruction plus Classroom Survival Strategies group scores showed a nonsignificant decline from a standard score of 76 to a standard score of 74. The final results of this comprehensive multidimensional study should yield helpful information about the response of the children's various cognitive and linguistic characteristics, including rapid naming, to differing linguistic interventions (semantic, morphologic, orthographic) with a core phonologic component.

In summary, these phonologically driven linguistic treatment studies indicate that the younger the child, the more explicit the intervention must be; the older the child and the more severe the impairment, the more intensive the treatment, and the longer its duration must be. A systematic phonics approach results in robust results in word reading accuracy but is not effective in developing fluency in the older, more impaired reader. Further research is needed to establish more effective interventions that will enable these children to become skilled readers.

Neuroimaging Studies and Response to Treatment
Neuroimaging studies have demonstrated the impact of intensive and explicit phonologically based remedial interventions on cortical activation. Simos and colleagues used magnetic source imaging to evaluate changes in spatiotemporal brain activation profiles while children performed a pseudoword reading task before and after treatment.[25] Treatment consisted of 8 weeks of intensive (one on one, 1 to 2 hours/day), phonologically based interventions in eight children aged 7 to 17 years (mean age 11.4 years) with dyslexia. Six of the eight children scored below the 4th percentile, and two scored at or below the 18th percentile on reading measures (the Basic Reading Skills cluster of the Woodcock-Johnson Test of Achievement, Third Edition.)[13] Intelligence was in the average range (mean IQ 102 ± 4.5 on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Third Edition [WISC-III]).[14] Six of the eight were diagnosed with attention-deficit disorder and were treated with psychostimulant medication throughout the study period. The comparison group consisted of eight children aged 8 to 14.2 years old (mean age 10.3 years) with reading scores above the 50th percentile and a mean IQ score of 107 (+ 10.5). One of the eight was diagnosed with attention-deficit disorder and was treated with medication. All 16 subjects were right-handed, native English speakers. Following intervention, reading accuracy in all subjects was above the 37th percentile. On the imaging studies prior to intervention, the subjects with dyslexia displayed the characteristic cortical activation profile of the individual with dyslexia.little to no activation in the left temporoparietal areas and strong activation in the homotopic right hemisphere. In contrast, the controls had little activation on the right and strongly activated the temporoparietal areas on the left, believed to be involved in phonologic processing. After treatment, the cortical activation patterns of the subjects with dyslexia resembled much more closely those of the normal controls. A dramatic increase was noted on the left, most pronounced in the left superior temporal gyrus, and increased activation in the inferior parietal areas approached significance. There was a moderate decrease in homotopic right hemisphere activation. The cortical activation patterns of the normal controls did not change. Of note, the imaging study also revealed that the subjects with dyslexia engaged the left superior temporal gyrus more slowly than did the controls, despite remediation, suggesting that the new circuitry might not be as efficient, and fluency would remain a problem. The authors stated that the small sample size calls for caution in interpreting the results. However, with the significant improvement noted both behaviorally and physiologically, they suggested that these findings indicate a "normalization" of functional brain organization following intensive intervention.

These findings have been replicated in a methodologically rigorous longitudinal study by Shaywitz and colleagues.[26] A large cohort (77 6- to 9-year-old children) with reading disabilities received 86 to 115 hours of daily, individual, and evidence-based phonologically mediated reading intervention at school. There were two control groups.a group of reading-disabled children who received the typical interventions of the school and private tutoring and a group of normal readers. The children were assessed before, following, and 1 year after treatment ended. Behavioral gains on the Gray Oral Reading Test, Third Edition, were found to be significant in comparison with the control reading-disabled group and the normal readers immediately post-treatment. The scale score improved from 5.4 to 7.0 for the treatment group; the scale score decreased from 5.4 to 4.9 for the reading-disabled control group on the passage score, a combination of accuracy and rate (mean 7, SD ±3). There could be no comparison at 1 year because only two of the reading-disabled control group and two of the normal reading group returned. The treatment group evidenced a decline in performance from a standard score of 7.0 to 6.4. However, an effect size calculation of the group's gains during treatment and of gains maintained from pretreatment to 1-year follow-up revealed an effect for both periods (effect size 0.52 and 0.43, respectively). Effect size in the 0.5 to 0.79 range is considered moderate.[27] Of note, the mean reading comprehension as measured by the Gray Oral Reading Test, Third Edition, improved during the treatment and at the 1-year follow-up, moving into and staying in the average range (pretest mean standard score 5.7; post-test mean standard score 8.0; standard score at 1-year follow-up 8.5). Other studies have demonstrated a greater ability with text comprehension than with word-level reading before treatment[11] and that, following treatment, comprehension exceeds the 30th percentile, although word reading ability still lags behind in the 13th to 22nd percentile. This suggests that enhanced phonologic decoding allows the person with dyslexia to better use other top-down processing strategies, which can be strengths.

Physiologic measures also revealed significant changes. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) during a letter identification task, Shaywitz et al. also demonstrated the shift from right hemisphere to increased left hemisphere activation reported by Simos et al.[25,26] Interestingly, although the treatment subjects remained in the impaired range on reading measures (Gray Oral Reading Test, Third Edition, passage score.5.4 preintervention, 7.0 immediately postintervention, and 6.4 1 year later) in contrast to the normal readers, both groups demonstrated similar brain activation patterns, with increased activation in the left inferior gyrus and posterior middle temporal gyrus immediately after treatment. At the 1-year follow-up, only the treatment group underwent a repeated imaging study, which revealed that these children activated the fast-paced occipitotemporal word form area serving skilled reading, the bilateral inferior frontal gyri, and left superior temporal regions.

Richards et al. used magnetic resonance spectroscopy (specifically, proton echo-planar spectroscopy) to evaluate the response to a less intensive but phonologically based intervention on brain lactate metabolism during "reading-related tasks" in eight boys with dyslexia and seven control boys between the ages of 10 and 13 years.[28] After the 3-week intervention period, which consisted of 15 2-hour group sessions, performance on behavioral measures of phonologic processing improved. Prior to intervention, the dyslexic participants manifested significantly greater lactate metabolism in the anterior quadrant of the left hemisphere during a reading task compared with controls, suggesting that they had greater difficulty with the task that necessitated increased use of the frontal cortex. One year after intervention, repeat magnetic resonance spectroscopy revealed a metabolic pattern similar to that of controls during the phonologic task.
To evaluate the effects of treatment using two different linguistic interventions, phonologic and morphologic, Berninger et al. conducted a study with children in fourth to sixth grade who were part of a family genetics study and were found to be dyslexic.[29] The children were of normal intelligence (mean Wechsler Verbal IQ was 110.6 ± 11), and although they did not have oral language problems or attention deficit, rapid naming deficits were present. The subjects were randomly assigned to a phonologic awareness or a morphologic awareness treatment group. They received daily treatment amounting to 28 hours over a period of 3 weeks. The behavioral results revealed significant gains in phonemic decoding skill and the rate from pretest to post-test for both groups. Phonologic decoding (the Woodcock-Johnson Reading Mastery Test Word Attack subtest) improved significantly for both groups. Interestingly, the morphology treatment group showed a significantly greater level of improvement than the phonology treatment group in phonologic decoding efficiency, as measured by the Pseudoword Reading Efficiency subtest of the Test of Word Reading Efficiency.[30] Berninger et al. suggested that the efficiency of phonologic decoding depends on an interaction between phonologic, morphologic, and orthographic representations, as described by Harm and Seidenberg's computer simulation studies below.[31] Moreover, following morphology treatment, magnetic resonance spectroscopy revealed decreased lactate activation in the left frontal region during a rhyming task in all but one child with dyslexia, and the activation pattern resembled that of normal readers. However, in the phonologic treatment group, the pattern of activation remained the same or increased in all of the subjects. A reduction in lactate activation might reflect increased efficiency of mental processing. Good readers have less activation in the left frontal region during this phonologic judgment task, indicating less need to enlist prefrontal executive resources. A second imaging study using functional MRI revealed that normally reading controls showed different patterns of activation when performing phonologic and morphologic judgment tasks that were stable over time. In contrast, the pretreatment activation patterns of the subjects with dyslexia differed from those of the controls but resembled them after treatment.

The study groups were small, and the intervention was brief; therefore, the interpretation of these results merits caution. Berninger et al. suggested that morphologic awareness training can help children develop the coordination for all of the language codes needed for word learning.[29] If this is so, they suggest that morphology provides a built-in executive function to do so. They recommend that future treatment design not only be aimed at orthographic and phonologic awareness but should also include training in morphologic awareness and the interrelationship of the three word forms.phonologic, morphologic, and orthographic.

Research Relating to Linguistically Based Commercial Programs
When classroom-based programs are not sufficient, clinicians and teachers can turn to commercial treatment programs. Many commercial programs (Alphabetic Phonics, Project Read, the Herman Approach, Slingerland Approach, the Spalding Approach, the Wilson Approach, LANGUAGE! [see Appendix]) are based on the Orton-Gillingham approach.[32] This is a multisensory explicit phonics method with emphasis on visual and auditory feedback for sounds and the tactile-kinesthetic input of letter formation. This evidence-based treatment is the only method offering a complete intervention, including writing instruction, and has been the forerunner in the field. Unfortunately, there are few methodologically sound studies in peer-reviewed journals to validate its efficacy. Quasiexperimental studies were published by the International Multisensory Structured Language Education Council in 1995.[33] Many of the studies were hampered by either large, diverse groups of subjects or by inadequate numbers of subjects, by gains being reported in grade equivalents and not standard scores, by no long-term follow-up, and by the absence of control groups.

A study by Guyer and colleagues using the Wilson Reading System to treat the spelling deficits of college students is of interest, although the sample size was small.[34] There were three groups of 10 subjects.a no-treatment control group, a group receiving intervention via the Wilson Reading System spelling approach, and a third group receiving treatment via a nonphonetic spelling approach. After 2 hours/week for 16 weeks, the students receiving the Wilson Reading System manifested significant gains in contrast to the other groups; spelling scores on the Wide Range Achievement Test-Revised[35] improved from a standard score of 76.7 to a score of 91, reaching the 30th percentile benchmark. The other groups did not show this level of improvement.
The results of Project Read interventions with young at-risk children and matched control groups in three school districts, using standard score gain measures to monitor gains, were described by Greene.[36] The treatment groups gained an annual mean of 12.7 standard score points (almost 1 SD) in 1 year, in contrast to the nonsignificant mean loss of 1.50 standard score points by matched-pair subjects in the control group. First-graders made the greatest gains in comparison with second- and third-graders.

In another study using the Orton-Gillingham method, Maskel and Felton evaluated the response of 230 older reading-disabled children (elementary through high school age) in a school for learning disabilities.[37] Subjects were instructed in small-group (3:1 or 4:1), daily (3 hours) sessions over 1 to 3 years, and the response was monitored over a 5-year period. The mean Wechsler Full-Scale IQ was 108. The subjects fell into the mildly impaired range.reading comprehension scores for all three groups were in the 100 to 104 range, and word reading skills were in the low 90s, based on the Woodcock Reading Mastery Test-Revised[38] and the Woodcock Johnson Tests of Achievement-Revised.[13] Based on the Wide Range Achievement Test-Revised,[35] pretest spelling scores were in the mid- to upper 80s. Significant gains were noted for all groups in comprehension and word-level reading. The subjects who made the greatest gains were those who required only 1 or 2 years of treatment. Spelling gains were significant for all except the middle and high school students who required 2 years of intervention, even though their baseline spelling scores were in the 35th to 40th percentile. The students requiring 3 years of intervention did not achieve significant gains in word identification or spelling despite beginning with relatively mild impairments. Their resistance to treatment would suggest that other factors were involved in their lack of response, but no other measures were available in the study. There was no control group.

These studies support the use of the Orton-Gillingham method, but future research using methodologically sound designs is badly needed. Two Orton-Gillingham interventions have been reported in peer-reviewed journals. Oakland et al. reported on a scientifically controlled study of the effectiveness of Alphabetic Phonics.[39,40] Their study measured word identification and comprehension ability after 350 hours of small-group treatment. There were moderate gains in word reading (standard score 72 to 82) and comprehension (13th to 22nd percentile; no standard scores were given), but the 30th percentile benchmark was not achieved. A study by Hook et al. revealed that an Orton-Gillingham approach resulted in significant gains in phonologic decoding after 6 weeks, in contrast to a group receiving FastForWord.[41]

The Lindamood Phoneme Sequencing program[12] has been described above and has been scientifically validated in a series of well-designed research studies.[8,11] The Phono-Graphix program also involves a systematic and explicit phonologic intervention but has less multisensory mediation.[42] It was used in the study by Simos and colleagues described above and resulted in significant behavioral and physiologic gains in the six patients who were treated.[25] However, no other scientifically controlled studies on this program have been reported. A word of caution is necessary.the Simos et al. study delivered the intervention in a one-to-one daily intensive manner using a trained teacher. However, the program was designed to be carried out by parents at home, with 1 to 2 hours per week of teacher support, making the intensity considerably less. No reliable study has validated this form of delivery. Intensity is a key ingredient to successful remediation, and the intensity needs to increase when the deficit is severe. Home programs in this day and age in families in which both parents are working are not realistic if intensity is required.

Nonlinguistic Interventions
The remedial intervention studies discussed thus far have been linguistically driven. Individuals with dyslexia have been reported to have co-occurring nonlinguistic deficits as well. Heilman and Alexander summarized the most commonly reported and considered them to be deterrents to the efficient mapping of phonemic representations, which are essential as the building blocks for a strong phonologic system.[43] These nonlinguistic road blocks include.a) impaired temporal processing of rapidly presented brief auditory, visual, and somatosensory stimuli; b) a slowed response time across domains, which is thought to compromise working memory with its time-limited capacity; and c) an attention deficit compromising working memory as well as executive functions and sustained attention. Children with dyslexia can evidence poor sensorimotor coordination requiring bimanual and/or rhythmic output[44] ; poor postural stability and low tone in the upper body and labored acquisition of skilled motor tasks, such as shoe tying and handwriting[45]; and difficulty with temporal sequential ordering in short-term auditory memory and working memory.[46] Subtle impairments in visual function include impaired binocular vergence control, with inefficient fixation on near targets and less accurate localization of small dots on a screen; difficulty recognizing several visual items presented simultaneously; trouble with letters or words "swimming" or jumping around on the page; and trouble with left-right discrimination.[47]

Monday, April 04, 2005

Azalea/Rhododendron Show April 16th 12:00 Atlanta Botanical Gardens

Off-topic - I would like to invite everyone to our American Rhododendron Society - Azalea Chapter annual Flower Show & Plant Sale at the Atlanta Botanical Gardens on April 16th 12:30 - late afternoon. I've posted more details and links below.

This is a great outing for everyone, including children and adults with disabilities. They love the garden and flowers. Take a tour of the Botanical Gardens while you come and see the Azalea Show. They have a spectacular children's garden!!

It's not too late to join, and put some clippings from your yard in the show as well!! We can teach you how!!

Carol Sadler
Special Education Consultant/Advocate
GA Advocacy Office PLSP I Graduate
770-442-8357
1105 Rock Pointe Look
Woodstock, GA 30188
CarolSadler@bellsouth.net
www.IEPadvocate4You.com
http://iepadvocate4you.blogspot.com/


Show 2004 - Carol Sadler and her Container Plant entries - Red Native won Best Container Plant
http://www.azaleachapter.com/2004Show/images/ACShow_36.JPG

Show 2004 - Flower entries
http://www.azaleachapter.com/2004Show/images/ACShow_6.JPG



AZALEA CHAPTER OF THE AMERICAN RHODODENDRON SOCIETY
www.azaleachapter.com
Chapter meetings and activities are open to the public at no charge. Membership is $35 and includes a chapter newsletter and quarterly journal. For more information visit their web site at http://azaleachapter.com - Be sure to designate the Atlanta, GA Azalea Chapter

Saturday, April 16. 12:00 Noon - 5:00 PM, The Exhibit Hall, Atlanta Botanical Garden, 1345 Piedmont Avenue, Atlanta
driving directions to the Atlanta Botanical Garden http://www.atlantabotanicalgarden.org/directions.htm

The Azalea Chapter Annual Flower Show. Dr. Joe Coleman, Chairman, Flower Show Committee
The Azalea Chapter Annual Plant Sale, Hugh Denny, Chairman, Plant Sale Committee

Program at 2:00 PM in Arts and Crafts Room: Steve Yeatts,
"In Search of Native Rhododendron and other Native Plants"

The Chapter Annual Flower Show truly brings out the best of the Chapter membership. Our members select the finest blooms from their gardens, shape the material for presentation and present the results at the show for others to enjoy.

Joe Coleman, a master rhododendron specialist is in charge of this year’s show. Joe and wife, Donna, won the Best in Show award last year and past flower show tradition strongly suggests that such winners serve as the show coordinator the next year. What a wonderful tradition and how thankful we are to have Joe and Donna in charge of this year’s show.

The Chapter urges you to consider participating in the show by exhibiting some your rhododendron or azaleas. If needed, assistance will be given to those not accustomed to exhibiting. Contact Joe Coleman (770-482-1289 or jcoleazalea@hotmail.com) for answers to questions and directions on bringing materials to the show.

Hugh Denny (404-633-9363 or hugh.denny@gtri.gatech.edu) is in charge of the Chapter Plant Sale held at the same time as the Flower Show. Members are encouraged to donate plants to the sale and to help support the Chapter. Contributions of azaleas, rhododendron and other plants are eagerly sought. Chapter members who support the show or sale are exempt from paying the normal ABG admission charges. When entering the garden, explain to the attendant that you are helping with the Show or Sale.

At 2:00 PM, there will be a presentation by member Steve Yeatts. Steve Yeatts, Azalea Chapter member and noted plantsman will share some of his experiences in hunting for various native plants throughout the Southeast. Steve's particular interests are native azaleas and trilliums. Steve and other noted associates conduct extensive plant search trips every year, traveling to various locations. Steve will present a power point presentation highlighting many of these trips and the plants found.


Another great GA plant association to join:
Georgia Native Plant Society - www.gnps.org
membership http://www.gnps.org/MEMBSHP2.HTM
They have monthly rescue digs around the greater Atlanta area. Join and help us save native GA plants! We rescue native azalea's and other native plants in the wild on weekly outings.

NOTE: If you have have property that is going to be developed, please contact the GA Native Plant Society and let us come and rescue the native plants on the site before they are demolished.


Feel free to re-distribute this e-mail to others!!